Facet Theory
   HOME

TheInfoList



OR:

Facet theory is a
metatheory A metatheory or meta-theory is a theory whose subject matter is theory itself, aiming to describe existing theory in a systematic way. In mathematics and mathematical logic, a metatheory is a mathematical theory about another mathematical theory. ...
for the multivariate
behavioral sciences Behavioral sciences explore the cognitive processes within organisms and the behavioral interactions between organisms in the natural world. It involves the systematic analysis and investigation of human and animal behavior through naturalistic o ...
that posits that
scientific theories A scientific theory is an explanation of an aspect of the natural world and universe that has been repeatedly tested and corroborated in accordance with the scientific method, using accepted protocols of observation, measurement, and evaluation ...
and
measurement Measurement is the quantification of attributes of an object or event, which can be used to compare with other objects or events. In other words, measurement is a process of determining how large or small a physical quantity is as compared ...
s can be advanced by discovering relationships between ''conceptual classifications'' of research variables and ''empirical partitions'' of data-representation spaces. For this purpose, facet theory proposes procedures for (1) Constructing or selecting variables for observation, using the ''mapping sentence'' technique (a formal definitional framework for a system of observations), and (2) Analyzing multivariate data, using data representation spaces, notably those depicting similarity measures (e.g.,
correlations In statistics, correlation or dependence is any statistical relationship, whether causal or not, between two random variable A random variable (also called random quantity, aleatory variable, or stochastic variable) is a mathematical formal ...
), or
partially ordered sets In mathematics, especially order theory, a partially ordered set (also poset) formalizes and generalizes the intuitive concept of an ordering, sequencing, or arrangement of the elements of a set. A poset consists of a set together with a binar ...
, derived from the data. Facet theory is characterized by its direct concern with the entire content-universe under study, containing many, possibly infinitely many, variables. Observed variables are regarded just as a sample of statistical units from the multitude of variables that make up the investigated attribute (the ''content-universe''). Hence, Facet theory proposes techniques for ''sampling'' variables for observation from the entire content universe; and for ''making inferences'' from the sample of observed variables to the entire content universe. The sampling of variables is done with the aid of the mapping sentence technique (see Section 1); and inferences from the sample of observed variables to the entire content universe are made with respect to correspondences between conceptual classifications (of attribute-variables or of population-members) and partitions of empirical geometric representation spaces obtained in data analysis (see Sections 2 & 3). Of the many types of representation spaces that have been proposed, two stand out as especially fruitful: ''Faceted-SSA (Faceted Smallest Space Analysis)'' for structuring the investigated attribute (see Section 2); and ''POSAC (Partial Order Scalogram Analysis by base Coordinates)'' for multiple scaling measurements of the investigated attribute (see Section 3).  Inasmuch as observed variables in a behavioral study form in fact but a sample from the content-universe of interest, facet theory's procedures and principles serve to avoid errors that may ensue from incidental sampling of observed variables, thus meeting the challenge of the
replication crisis The replication crisis (also called the replicability crisis and the reproducibility crisis) is an ongoing methodological crisis in which the results of many scientific studies are difficult or impossible to reproduce. Because the reproducibili ...
in psychological research and in behavioral research in general. Facet Theory was initiated by
Louis Guttman Louis (Eliyahu) Guttman (February 10, 1916 – October 25, 1987; he, לואיס (אליהו) גוטמן) was an American sociologist and Professor of Social and Psychological Assessment at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, known primarily for ...
and has been further developed and applied in a variety of disciplines of the
behavioral sciences Behavioral sciences explore the cognitive processes within organisms and the behavioral interactions between organisms in the natural world. It involves the systematic analysis and investigation of human and animal behavior through naturalistic o ...
including
psychology Psychology is the scientific study of mind and behavior. Psychology includes the study of conscious and unconscious phenomena, including feelings and thoughts. It is an academic discipline of immense scope, crossing the boundaries betwe ...
,
sociology Sociology is a social science that focuses on society, human social behavior, patterns of Interpersonal ties, social relationships, social interaction, and aspects of culture associated with everyday life. It uses various methods of Empirical ...
, and
business administration Business administration, also known as business management, is the administration of a commercial enterprise. It includes all aspects of overseeing and supervising the business operations of an organization. From the point of view of management ...
.


The mapping sentence


Definition and properties of the mapping sentence

Definition (Guttman). A ''mapping sentence'' is a verbal statement of the domain and of the range of a mapping including connectives between facets as in ordinary language. In the context of behavioral research, a mapping sentence is essentially a function whose domain consists of the respondents and of the stimuli as arguments, and whose image consists of the
cartesian product In mathematics, specifically set theory, the Cartesian product of two sets ''A'' and ''B'', denoted ''A''×''B'', is the set of all ordered pairs where ''a'' is in ''A'' and ''b'' is in ''B''. In terms of set-builder notation, that is : A\ti ...
of the ranges of responses to the stimuli, where each response-range is similarly ordered from high to low with respect to a concept common to all stimuli. When stimuli are classified a priori by one or more content criteria, the mapping sentence facilitates stratified sampling of the content-universe. A classification of the stimuli by their content is called a ''content facet''; and the pre-specified set of responses to a stimulus (classifying respondents by their response to that stimulus) is called a ''range facet''. The mapping sentence defines the system of observations to be performed. As such, the mapping sentence provides also the essential concepts in terms of which research hypotheses may be formulated.


An example from intelligence research

Suppose members ''pi'' of a population P are observed with respect to their success in a written verbal intelligence test. Such observations may be described as a mapping from the observed population to the set of possible scores, say, ''R'' = : ''P''''q''1 → ''R'', where ''q''1 is the sense in which a specific score is assigned to every individual in the observed population ''P'', i.e., ''q''1 is "verbal intelligence" in this example. Now, one may be interested in observing also the mathematical or, more specifically, the numerical intelligence of the investigated population; and possibly also their spatial intelligence. Each of these kinds of intelligence is a "sense" in which population members ''pi'' may be mapped into a range of scores ''R'' = . Thus, 'intelligence' is now differentiated into three types of materials: verbal (''q''1), numerical (''q''2) and spatial (''q''3). Together, ''P'', the population, and ''Q'' = , the set of types of intelligence, form a cartesian product which constitutes the mapping domain. The mapping is from the set of pairs (pi, qj) to the common range of test-scores ''R'' = : ''P'' × ''Q'' → ''R''. A ''facet'' is a set that serves as a component-set of a cartesian product. Thus, ''P'' is called the ''population facet'', ''Q'' is called a ''content facet,'' and the set of scores obtainable for each test is a ''range facet''. The range facets of the various items (variables) need not be identical in size: they may have any finite number of scores, or categories, greater or equal to 2.


The Common Meaning Range (CMR)

The ranges of the items pertaining to an investigated content-universe – intelligence in this example – should all have a Common Meaning Range (CMR); that is, they must be ordered from high to low with respect to a common meaning. Following Guttman, the common meaning proposed for the ranges of intelligence-items is "correctness with respect to an objective rule". The concept of CMR is central in facet theory: It serves to define the content-universe being studied by specifying the universe of items pertaining to that content-universe. Thus, the ''mapping-definition'' of intelligence, advanced by facet theory is: "An item belongs to the universe of intelligence items if and only if its domain requires performance of a cognitive task concerning an objective rule and its range is ordered from high correctness to low correctness with respect to that rule." An initial framework for observing intelligence could be Mapping Sentence 1. The mapping sentence serves as a unified semantic device for specifying the system of intelligence test items, according to the present conceptualization. Its content facet, the material facet, may now serve as a classification of intelligence test items to be considered. Thus, in designing observations, a stratified sampling of items is afforded by ensuring an appropriate selection of items from each of the material facet elements; that is, from each class of items: the verbal, the numerical and the spatial.


Enriching the mapping sentence

The research design can be enriched by introducing to the mapping sentence an additional, independent classification of the observations in the form of an additional content-facet, thereby facilitating systematic differentiations of the observations. For example, intelligence items may be classified also according to the cognitive operation required in order to respond correctly to an item: whether rule-recall (memory), rule-application, or rule-inference. Instead of the three sub-content-universes of intelligence defined by the material facet alone, we now have nine sub-content-universes defined by the cartesian multiplication of the material and the mental-operation facets. See mapping sentence 2. Another way of enriching a mapping sentence (and the scope of the research) is by adding an element (a class) to an existing content facet; for example, by adding Interpersonal material as a new element to the extant material facet. See Mapping Sentence 3.


Content profiles

A selection of one element from each of the two content facets defines a content profile which represents a sub-content-universe of
intelligence Intelligence has been defined in many ways: the capacity for abstraction, logic, understanding, self-awareness, learning, emotional knowledge, reasoning, planning, creativity, critical thinking, and problem-solving. More generally, it can b ...
. For example, the content profile (''c2, q2'') represents the application of rules for performing mathematical computations, such as performing long division. The 3x4=12 sub-content-universes constitute twelve classes of intelligence items. In designing observations, the researcher would strive to include a number of varied items from each of these 12 classes so that the sample of observed items would be representative of the entire intelligence universe. Of course, this stratified sampling of items depends on the researchers' conception of the studied domain, reflected in their choice of content-facets. But, in the larger cycle of the scientific investigation (which includes Faceted SSA of empirical data, see next section), this conception may undergo adjustments and remolding, converging to improved choices of content-facets and observations, and ultimately to robust theories in research domain. In general, mapping sentences may attain high levels of complexity, size and abstraction through various logical operations such as recursion, twist, decomposition and completion.


Cartesian decomposition and completion: an example

In drafting a mapping sentence, an effort is made to include the most salient content-facets, according to the researcher's existing conception of the investigated domain. And for each content facet, attempt is made to specify its elements (classes) so that they be exhaustive (complete) and exclusive (non-overlapping) of each other. Thus, the element 'interpersonal' has been added to the incumbent 3-element material facet of intelligence by a two-step facet-analytic procedure. Step 1, cartesian decomposition of the 3-element material facet into two binary elementary facets: The Environment Facet, whose elements are 'physical environment' and 'human-environment'; and the Symbolization Facet whose elements are 'symbolic' (or high symbolization), and 'concrete' (or low symbolization). Step 2, cartesian completion of the material facet is then sought by attempting to infer the missing material classifiable as 'human environment' and 'concrete'. In facet theory, this 2×2 classification of intelligence-testing material may now be formulated as an hypothesis to be tested empirically, using Faceted Smallest Space Analysis (SSA).


Complementary topics concerning the mapping sentence

Despite its seemingly rigid appearance, the mapping sentence format can accommodate complex semantic structures such as twists and recursions, while retaining its essential cartesian structure. In addition to guiding the collection of data, mapping sentences have been used to content-analyze varieties of conceptualizations and texts—such as organizational quality, legal documents and even dream stories.


Concepts as spaces: faceted SSA


Description of Faceted Smallest Space Analysis (Faceted SSA)

Facet theory conceives of a multivariate attribute as a content-universe defined by the set of all its items, as specified by the attribute mapping-definition, illustrated above. In facet-theoretical data analysis, the attribute (e.g., intelligence) is likened to a geometric space of suitable dimensionality, whose points represent all possible items. Observed items are processed by Faceted SSA, a version of Multidmensional Scaling (MDS) which involves the following steps: # Receiving as input (or computing from input data) a matrix of similarity coefficients, specifying, for each pair of items how similar they are. A common example is the computation of a correlation-coefficient matrix from input data, where the size of a correlation coefficient between two variables reflects the degree of similarity between them. # Mapping the items (variables) as points in a geometric space of a given dimensionality while preserving as well as possible the condition: If ''rij>rkl'' then ''dij''<''dkl'' for all ''i,j,k,l'' where ''rij'' is the similarity measure (e.g., correlation coefficient) between variables ''i,j'' and ''dij'' is the distance between their points in the space. Most often, Euclidean distance function (Minkowsky distance of order 2) is used. But other distance functions, especially the Manhattan distance function (Minkowsky distance of order 1) are called for. (See Subsection Relating POSAC Measurement Space to the SSA Concept Space below.) The goodness-of-fit of the resulting mapping may be assessed by a loss function– Kruskal's Stress coefficient or Guttman's Coefficient of Alienation. # Partitioning the space as well as possible, into simple regions (stripe, sectors or concentric rings) whose variables are in 1-1 correspondence with a pre-conceived content-facet. To run this option, content facet(s) must be specified as Faceted SSA input. Step 3 of Faceted SSA incorporates the idea that observed variables included in the Faceted SSA procedure, typically constitute a small subset from the countless items that define the attribute content-universe. But their locations in space may serve as clues that guide the partitioning of the space into regions, in effect classifying all points in space, including those pertaining to unobserved items (had they been observed). This procedure, then, tests the regional hypothesis that the sub-content-universes defined by a content-facet elements exist each as a distinct empirical entity. The Shye-Kingsley Separation Index (SI) assesses the goodness-of-fit of the partition to the content-facet.Borg, I & Shye, S. (1995). ''Facet Theory: Form and Content''. Thousand Oaks CA: Sage, pp. 143–146. The spatial scientific imagery suggested by Facet Theory has far reaching consequences that set Facet Theory apart from other statistical procedures and research strategies. Specifically, it facilitates inferences concerning the structure of the entire content-universe investigated, including unobserved items.


Example 1. The structure of intelligence

Intelligence testing has been conceived as described above, with Mapping Sentence 2 as a framework for its observation. In many studies, different samples of variables conforming to Mapping Sentence 2 have been analyzed confirming two regional hypotheses: * The Material Content Facet corresponds to a partition of the Faceted SSA map of intelligence into sectors, each containing the items of a single material — verbal, numeric, and figural (spatial). * The Cognitive Operation Facet corresponds to a partition of the Faceted SSA map of intelligence into concentric rings, with the innermost ring containing inference items; the middle ring containing the rule-application items; and the outermost ring containing the rule-recall items. The superposition of these two partition patterns results in a scheme known as the Radex Theory of Intelligence, see Figure 1. The radex structure, which originated earlier as "a new approach to factor analysis", has been found also in the study of color perceptionShepard, R. N. (1978). The circumplex and related topological manifolds in the study of perception. In S. Shye (Ed.), ''Theory construction and data analysis in the behavioral sciences'' (pp. 29-80). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. (A volume in honor of Louis Guttman) as well as in other domains of research. Faceted SSA has been applied in a wide variety of research areas including value research social work and criminology and many others.


Example 2. The structure of quality of life

The Systemic Quality of Life (SQOL) has been defined as the effective functioning of human individuals in four functioning subsystems: the cultural, the social, the physical and the personality subsystems. The axiomatic foundations of SQOL suggest the regional hypothesis that the four subsystems should be empirically validated (i.e., item of each would occupy a distinct region) and that they be mutually oriented in space in a specific 2x2 pattern topologically equivalent to the 2x2 classification shown in Figure 2 (i.e., personality opposite cultural, and physical opposite social). The hypothesis has been confirmed by many studies.


Types of partition patterns

Of the many possible partitions of a 2-d concept space, three stand out as especially useful for theory construction: * The ''Axial'' Partition Pattern: Partitioning of the space into stripes by parallel lines. * The ''Angular'' (a/k/a ''polar'') Partition Pattern: Partitioning of the space into sectors by radii emanating from a point in space. * The ''Radial'' (a/k/a ''modular'') Partition Pattern: Partitioning of the space into concentric rings by concentric circles. The advantages of these partition patterns as likely models for behavioral data are that they are describable by a minimal number of parameters, hence avoid overfitting; and that they are generalizable to partition in spaces of higher dimensionalities. In testing regional hypotheses, the fit of a content-facet to any one of these three models is assessed the Separation Index (SI), a normalized measure of the deviation of variables from the region assigned to them by the model. Concept spaces in higher dimensionalities have been found as well.


Principles of faceted SSA: A summary

1. The attribute under study is represented by a geometric space. 2. Variables of the attribute are represented as points in that space. Conversely, every point in the geometric space is a variable of the attribute. This is the Continuity Principle. 3. The observed variables, located as points in the empirical Faceted SSA map, constitute but a sample drawn from the many (possibly infinitely many) variables constituting the content universe of the attribute investigated. 4. The observed variables chosen for SSA must all belong to the same content universe. This is ensured by including in the SSA only variables whose ranges are similarly ordered with respect to a common meaning (CMR). 5. The sample of variables marked on the Faceted SSA map is used as a guide for inferring possible partitions of the SSA-attribute-map into distinct regions, each region representing a component, or subdomain, of the attribute. 6. In Facet Theory, relationships between attribute components (such as verbal intelligence and numeric intelligence as components of intelligence), are expressed in geometric terms –such as shapes and spatial orientation – rather than in algebraic terms. Just as one would describe relationships between neighboring countries in terms of their shapes and geographical orientation, not in terms of distances between them. 7. The imagery of an attribute as a continuous space, from which variables are sampled, implies that clustering of variables in SSA map has no significance: It is just an artifact of the sampling of the variables. Sampled variables that are clustered together may belong to different subdomains; just as two cities that are close together may be located in different countries. Conversely, variables that are far apart, may belong to the same sub-domain; just as two cities that are far apart may belong to the same country. What matters is the identification of distinct regions with well-defined sub-domains. Facet Theory proposes a way of transcending accidental clustering of variables by focusing on a robust and replicable aspect of the data, namely the partitionability of the attribute-space. These principles bring in new concepts, raise new questions, and opens new ways of understanding behavior. Thus, Facet Theory represents a paradigm of its own for multivariate behavioral research.


Complementary topics in faceted SSA

Besides analyzing a data matrix of ''N'' individuals by ''n'' variables, as discussed above, Faceted SSA is usefully employed in additional modes. Direct measures of (dis)similarity. For a given a set of objects and a similarity (or dissimilarity) measure between every pair of objects, Faceted SSA can provide a map whose regions correspond to a specified classification of the objects. For example, in a study of color perception, a sample of spectral colors, with a measure of perceived similarity between every pair of colors, yielded the radex theory of spectral color perception. In a study of community elites, a measure of distance devised between pairs of community leaders, yielded a sociometric map whose regions were interpreted from the perspective of sociological theory. Transposed data matrix. Switching the roles of individuals and variables, Faceted SSA may be applied to individuals rather than to the variables. This rarely used procedure may be justified to the extent variables evenly cover a research domain. For example, intercorrelations between members of a multidisciplinary team of experts were computed based on their human quality-of life value assessments. The resulting Faceted SSA map yielded a radex of disciplines, supporting the association between social institutions and human values.


Multiple scaling by POSAC


Description of Partial Order Scalogram Analysis by Coordinates (POSAC)

In Facet Theory, the measurement of investigated individuals (and, by extension, of all individuals belonging to the sampled population) with respect to a multivariate attribute, is based on the following assumptions and conditions: * Variables processed by Facet Theory measurement operations to be described below, evenly cover the attribute content universe. To ensure such coverage, Facet Theory measurement operations are often performed not on the sample of the observed items themselves, but rather on composite variables that represent facet elements that had been validated by Faceted SSA. * The sample of individuals is rich enough to allow existing score-profiles of the processed variables to be observed. * In the resulting measurement, order relations among individuals should preserve sufficiently well order relations (including comparability and incomparability; see below) between individuals' profiles of the processed variables. * The result of the measurement operation yields the smallest number of scales; * The resultant scales represent fundamental variables whose interpretation derives from the contents of the observed items, but does not depend on the particular sample of items observed. Partial order analysis of observed data''.'' Let observed items ''v1,...,vn'' with a common-meaning range (CMR) represent an investigated content universe; let ''A''1,...,''A''''n'' be their ranges with each ''Aj'' ordered from high to low with respect to the common meaning; and let ''A'' = ''A''1×''A''2 × ... × ''A''''n'' be the cartesian product of all the range facets, ''Aj'' (''j'' = 1,...,''n''). A system of observations is a mapping ''P'' → ''A'' from the observed subjects ''P'' to ''A'', that is, each subject ''p''''i'' gets a score from each ''A''''j'' (''j'' = 1,...,''n''), or ''p''''i'' →  'a''''i''1,''a''''i''2, ..., ''a''''in''\equiv ''a''(''pi''). The point ''a''(''p''''i'') in ''A'' is also called the profile of ''p''''i'', and the subset ''A''′ of ''A'' (A'\subseteq A) of observed profiles is called a scalogram. Facet Theory defines relations between profiles as follows: Two different profiles ''a''''i'' =  'a''''i''1,''a''''i''2,...,''a''''in''and ''a''''j'' =  'a''''j''1,''a''''j''2,...,''a''''jn'' are comparable, denoted by ''a''''i''''Sa''''j'', with ''a''''i'' greater than ''a''''j'', ''a''''i'' > ''a''''j'', if and only if ''a''''ik'' ≥ ''a''''jk'' for ''k'' = 1, ..., ''n'', and ''a''''ik''′ > ''a''''jk''′ for some ''k''. Two different profiles are incomparable, denoted by ''a''''i'' $ ''a''''j'', if neither ''a''''i'' > ''a''''j'' nor ''a''''j'' > ''a''''i''. ''A,'' and therefore its subset ''A''′, form a partially ordered set. Facet Theoretical measurement consists in mapping points ''a''(''pi'') of ''A into a coordinate space ''X'' of the lowest dimensionality while preserving observed order relations, including incomparability: Definition. The p.o. dimensionality of scalogram ''A''' is the smallest ''m'' (''m'' ≤ ''n'') for which there exist ''m'' facets ''X''1 ... ''X''''m'' (each ''Xi'' is ordered) and there exists a 1-1 mapping ''Q'':''X''′ → ''A''′ from ''X''′ (X'\subseteq X= X_1\times\cdots\times X_m) to ''A''′ such that ''a'' > ''a''′ if and only if ''x'' > ''x''′ whenever ''Q'' maps points ''x'', ''x''′ in ''X''′ to points ''a'', ''a''′ ∈ ''A''. The coordinate scales, ''Xi'' (''i'' = 1, ..., ''m'') represent underlying fundamental variables whose meanings must be inferred in any specific application. The well known Guttman scale 4(example: 1111, 1121, 1131, 2131, 2231, 2232) is simply a 1-d scalogram, i.e. one all of whose profiles are comparable. The procedure of identifying and interpreting the coordinate scales ''X''1...''X''''m'' is called multiple scaling. multiple scaling is facilitated by partial order scalogram analysis by base coordinates (POSAC) for which algorithms and computer programs have been devised. In practice, a particular dimensionality is attempted and a solution that best accommodates the order-preserving condition is sought. The POSAC/LSA program finds an optimal solution in 2-d coordinate space, then goes on to analyze by Lattice Space Analysis (LSA) the role played by each of the variables in structuring the POSAC 2-space, thereby facilitating interpretation of the derived coordinate scales, ''X''1, ''X''2. Recent developments include the algorithms for computerized partitioning of the POSAC space by the range facet of each variable, which induces meaningful intervals on the coordinate scales, ''X'', ''Y''.


Example 3. TV watching patterns: analysis of simplified survey data

Members of a particular population were asked four questions: whether they watched TV the night before for an hour at 7 PM (hour 1), at 8 PM (hour 2), at 9 PM (hour 3) and at 10 PM (hour 4). A positive answer to a question was recorded as 1, and a negative answer, as 0. Thus, for example, the profile 1010 represents a person who watched TV at 7 PM and at 9 PM but not at 8 PM and at 10 PM. Suppose that out of the 16 combinatorially possible profiles, only the following eleven profiles were observed empirically: 0000, 1000, 0100, 0010, 0001, 1100, 0110, 0011, 1110, 0111, 1111. Figure 3 is an order-preserving mapping of these profiles into a 2-dimensional coordinate space. Given this POSAC solution, an attempt is made to interpret the two coordinates, ''X''1 and ''X''2, as two fundamental scales of the investigated phenomenon of evening TV watching by the investigated population. This is done by, first, interpreting the intervals (equivalence classes) within each coordinate, and then trying to conceptualize the derived meanings of the ordered intervals, in terms of a meaningful notion that may be attributed to the coordinate. In the present simplified example, this is easy: Inspecting the map, we attempt to identify the feature that distinguishes all profiles with given score in ''X''1. Thus, we find that profiles with ''X1''=4, and only they, represent TV watching in the fourth hour. Profiles with ''X''1 = 3 all have 1 in the third watching hour but 0 at the fourth hour, i.e., the third hour is the latest watching hour. ''X''1 = 2 is assigned to, and only to, profiles whose latest watching hour is the second hour. And, finally, ''X''1 = 1 is for the profile 1000 which represents the fact that the first hour is the only – and therefore the latest – watching hour (ignoring the profile 0000 of those who didn’t watch TV at the specified hours, and could be assigned (0,0) in this coordinate-space). Hence, it may be concluded that intervals of coordinate ''X1'' represent j=the latest hour—among the four hours observed—in which TV was watched, (''j'' = 1, …, 4). Similarly, it is found that intervals of coordinate ''X''2 represent 5 − ''k'' for ''k'' (''k'' = 1, …, 4) is the earliest hour of TV watching. Indeed, for profiles of the observed set, which represent a single sequence of continuous TV watching, specification of the earliest and latest watching hours, provide full description of the watching hours. Example 3 illustrates key features of Multiple Scaling by POSAC that render this procedure a theory-based multivariate measurement: * The two scores assigned by Multiple Scaling to every observed profile—and hence to every person in the observed sample—replace the more numerous scores (four, in the present example) of the observed variables, while retaining all observed order relations, including incomparability. The new scores assess observed persons on the two coordinate-scales, taken to constitute Nature's fundamental variables. * The two coordinate-scales have intrinsic meanings that probe into a deeper significance than the observed variables considered severally. In the present example, the earliest and the latest hour indeed exhaust the essential aspects of the pattern of TV watching, given the particular set of observed profiles. * The concepts derived for the fundamental, unobserved coordinate-scales retain the CMR—the essential meaning common to all observed variables. In the present example, the CMR is more (vs. less) TV watching. For, considering the observed variables, each of them records high (1) vs. low (0) TV watching in a given hour. And the derived coordinate-scales, too, record high (4) vs. low (1) TV watching, since ceteris paribus, the later is the latest watching hour, the more TV one watches (''X''1); and the earlier is the earliest watching hour, the more TV one watches ( ''X''2). These features are present also in applications that are less obvious, to produce scales with novel meanings.


Example 4. Measuring distributive justice attitudes

In the systemic theory of distributive justice (DJ), alternative allocations of a given amount of an educational resource (100 supplementary teaching hours) between gifted and disadvantaged pupils, may be classified by one of four types, the preference for each reflecting one's DJ attitude: Equality, where the gifted and the disadvantaged pupils get the same amount of the supplementary resource; Fairness, where the disadvantaged pupils get more of the resource than the gifted, in proportion to their weakness relative to the gifted; Utility, where the gifted get more of the resource than the disadvantaged pupils (so as to promote future contribution to the general good); Corrective Action, where the disadvantaged pupils get more of the resource than the gifted over and above the proportion of their weakness relative to the gifted pupils, (so as to compensate them for past accumulated disadvantage); Following the Faceted SSA validation of the four DJ modes of Equality, Fairness, Utility, and Corrective Action, profiles based on eight dichotomized DJ attitudes variables observed on a sample of 191 respondents, were created. 35 of the 256 combinatorially possible profile were observed and analyzed by POSAC to obtain the measurement space shown in Figure 4. For each of the variables an optimal partition- line was computed that separates a high from a low score in that variable. (Logically, partition-lines must look like non-increasing step functions.) Then, for each of the four attitude types, the characteristic partition-line was identified as follows: Fairness—a straight vertical line; Utility—a straight horizontal line; Equality—an L-shaped line; Corrective action—an inverted-L-shaped line The content significance of the intervals induced by these partition-lines on the X coordinate and on the Y coordinate of the POSAC space, are now identified and thereby define the contents of the X and Y Coordinate Scales of DJ attitudes. The X-coordinate Scale, interpreted as ''Enhanced Fairness Attitude Scale:'' * Interval 1. Low Fairness & Low Equality DJ Attitude * Interval 2. Low Fairness & High Equality DJ Attitude * Interval 3. High Fairness & Low Corrective Action DJ Attitude * Interval 4. High Fairness & High Corrective Action DJ Attitude That is, Enhanced Fairness Attitude, even if low, (interval 1 and 2) is somewhat present when Equality is favored (interval 2). And if Enhanced Fairness Attitude is high (intervals 3 and 4), it reaches the extreme level (interval 4) when Corrective Action is favored. The Y-coordinate Scale, interpreted as ''Enhanced Utility Attitude Scale'': * Interval 1. Low Utility & Low Equality DJ Attitude * Interval 2. Low Utility & High Equality DJ Attitude * Interval 3. High Utility s & Low Corrective Action DJ Attitude * Interval 4. High Utility & High Corrective Action DJ Attitude That is, Enhanced Utility Attitude, even if low, (interval 1 and 2) is somewhat present when Equality is favored (interval 2). If Enhanced Utility Attitude is high (intervals 3 and 4), it reaches the extreme level (interval 4) when Corrective Action is favored. (This may well reflect the sentiment that, in the long run, the advancement of disadvantaged pupils serves the common good.) The meanings of the fundamental variables, X and Y, while relying on the concepts of fairness and of utility, respectively, suggest new notions that modify them. The new notions were christened Enhanced (or Extended) Fairness and Enhanced (or Extended) Utility.


Complementary topics in partial order spaces

''Higher order partition lines.'' The above simple measurement space illustrates partition-lines that are straight or have one bend. More complex measurement spaces result with items whose partition-lines have two or more bends. While partial order spaces are used mainly for analyzing score profiles (based on range facets), under certain conditions, they may be applied to the analysis of content profiles; i.e., those based on content facets. ''Relating POSAC Measurement Space to the SSA Concept Space.'' Based on the same data matrix, POSAC measurement space and Faceted SSA concept space are mathematically related. Proved relationships rely on the introduction of a new kind of coefficient, E*, the coefficient of structural similarity. While E* assesses pairwise similarity between variables, it does depend on variations in the remaining n-2 variables processed. That is, in the spirit of Facet Theory, E* depends on the sampled contents as well as on the sampled population. LSA1 procedure, within 2-dimensional POSAC/LSA program, is a special version of SSA with E* as the similarity coefficient, and with lattice ("city block") as the distance function. Under specified conditions, LSA1 may be readily derived from the boundary scales of the POSAC configuration, thereby highlighting concept/measurement space duality.


Facet theory: comparisons and comments

Concerned with the entire cycle of multivariate research – concept definition, observational design, and data analysis for concept-structure and measurement, Facet Theory constitutes a novel paradigm for the behavioral sciences. Hence, only limited aspects of it can be compared with specific statistical methods. A distinctive feature of Facet Theory is its explicit concern with the entire set of variables included in the investigated content-universe, regarding the subset of observed variables as but a sample from which inferences can be made. Hence, clusters of variables, if observed, are of no significance. They are simply unimportant artifacts of the procedure for sampling of the variables. This is in contrast with
cluster analysis Cluster analysis or clustering is the task of grouping a set of objects in such a way that objects in the same group (called a cluster) are more similar (in some sense) to each other than to those in other groups (clusters). It is a main task of ...
or
factor analysis Factor analysis is a statistical method used to describe variability among observed, correlated variables in terms of a potentially lower number of unobserved variables called factors. For example, it is possible that variations in six observed ...
where recorded clustering patterns determine research results and interpretations. There have been various attempts to describe technical differences between
Factor Analysis Factor analysis is a statistical method used to describe variability among observed, correlated variables in terms of a potentially lower number of unobserved variables called factors. For example, it is possible that variations in six observed ...
and Facet Theory. Briefly, it may be said that while
Factor Analysis Factor analysis is a statistical method used to describe variability among observed, correlated variables in terms of a potentially lower number of unobserved variables called factors. For example, it is possible that variations in six observed ...
aims to structure the set of variables selected for observation, Facet Theory aims to structure the entire content universe of all variables, observed as well as unobserved, relying on the continuity principle and using regional hypotheses as an inferential procedure. Guttman's SSA, as well as Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) in general, were often described as a procedure for visualizing similarities (e.g., correlations) between analyzed units (e.g., variables) in which the researcher has specific interest. (See, for example, Wikipedia, October 2020: "
Multidimensional scaling Multidimensional scaling (MDS) is a means of visualizing the level of similarity of individual cases of a dataset. MDS is used to translate "information about the pairwise 'distances' among a set of n objects or individuals" into a configurati ...
(MDS) is a means of visualizing the level of similarity of individual cases of a dataset"). Modern Facet Theory, however, concerned with theory construction in the behavioral sciences, assigns SSA/MDS space a different role. Regarding the analyzed units as a sample of statistical units representing all units that pertain to the content-universe, their dispersion in the SSA/MDS space is used to infer the structure of the content universe. Namely, to infer space partitionings that define components of the content-universes and their spatial interrelationships. The inferred structure, if replicated, may suggest a theory in the investigated domain and provide a basis for theory-based measurements. Misgivings and responses One reservation that has been voiced concerns the usefulness of a successful SSA map (one whose partition-pattern matches a content-classification of the mapped variables). What are the consequences of an SSA map? Does such a map qualify as a theory? In response, it may be pointed out that (a) consistently replicated empirical partition-patterns in a domain of research constitute a scientific lawfulness which, as such, are of interest to Science; (b) Often a partition-pattern leads to insights that explain behavior and may have potential applications. For example, the ''Radex Theory of Intelligence'' implies that inferential abilities are less differentiated by kinds of material than memory (or rule-recall, see Example 1 above). (c) Faceted SSA is a useful preliminary procedure for performing meaningful non arbitrary measurements by Multiple Scaling (POSAC). See Example 4. A common doubt about SSA was voiced by a sympathetic but mystified user of SSA: "Smallest Space Analysis seems to come up with provocative pictures that an imaginative observer can usually make some sense of –– in fact, I have often referred to SSA as the sociologist's Rorschach test for imagination". Indeed, missing in Facet Theory are statistical significance tests that would indicate the stability of discovered or hypothesized partition patterns across population samples. For example, it is not clear how to compute the probability of obtaining a hypothesized partition pattern, assuming that in fact the variables are randomly dispersed over the SSA map. In response, facet theorists claim that in Facet Theory the stability of research results is established by replications, as is the common practice in the natural sciences. Thus, if the same partition-pattern is observed across many population samples (and if no unexplained counterexamples are recorded), confidence in the research outcome would increase. Moreover, Facet Theory adds a stringent requirement for establishing scientific lawfulness, namely that the hypothesized partition-pattern would hold also across different selections of variables, sampled from the same mapping sentence. Facet Theory is regarded as a promising
metatheory A metatheory or meta-theory is a theory whose subject matter is theory itself, aiming to describe existing theory in a systematic way. In mathematics and mathematical logic, a metatheory is a mathematical theory about another mathematical theory. ...
for the behavioral sciences by
Clyde Coombs Clyde Hamilton Coombs (July 22, 1912 – February 4, 1988) was an American psychologist specializing in the field of Psychometrics, mathematical psychology. He devised a voting system, that was hence named Coombs' method. Coombs founded the Mathem ...
, an eminent
psychometrician Psychometrics is a field of study within psychology concerned with the theory and technique of measurement. Psychometrics generally refers to specialized fields within psychology and education devoted to testing, measurement, assessment, and ...
and pioneer of mathematical psychology, who commented: “It is not uncommon for a behavioral theory to be somewhat ambiguous about its domain. The result is that an experiment usually can be performed which will support it and another experiment will disconfirm it. … The problem of how to define the boundaries of a domain, especially in social and behavioral science, is subtle and complex. Guttman’s facet theory (see Shye, 1978) is, I believe, the only substantial attempt to provide a general theory for characterizing domains; in this sense, it is a metatheory. As behavioral science advances so will the need for such theory.”Coombs, C. H. (1983). ''Psychology and Mathematics: An Essay on Theory.'' Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press 1983.


References


Further reading

* Guttman, R. & Greenbaum, C. W. (1998). "Facet Theory: Its Development and Current Status." ''European Psychologist'', Vol. 3, No. 1, March 1998, pp. 13–36. * Levy, S. (Ed.) (1994). ''Louis Guttman on Theory and Methodology: Selected Writings.'' Aldershot: Dartmouth. * Canter (Ed.) (1985). Facet Theory: Approaches to Social Research. New York: Springer. * Guttman, R. (1994). Radex Theory. In Robert J. Sternberg (Ed.), ''Encyclopedia of Human Intelligence.'' New York, NY: Macmillan Publishing, 907–912. Behavioural sciences Data analysis Metatheory Order theory Philosophy of science